top of page

A widely known research article, by two psychologists from the University of Washington, confirms the view that, "questions asked subsequent to an event can cause a reconstruction in one’s memory of that event". Elizabeth F. Loftus & John C. Palmer, Reconstruction of Automobile Destruction: An Example of the Interaction Between Language and Memory, 13 Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 585, 585 (1974), available at: https://webfiles.uci.edu/eloftus/LoftusPalmer74.pdf .


This study tested whether or not how fast people would guess cars involved in an accident were going would change based on the verb used to describe the collision. Participants were asked to answer one of these five variations on a question about the cars' speed:


- About how fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other?

- About how fast were the cars going when they collided into each other?

- About how fast were the cars going when they bumped each other?

- About how fast were the cars going when they hit each other?

- About how fast were the cars going when they contacted each other?


The speed estimates varied directly with the strength of the word that was used:



A second set of people was shown films showing an auto accident, and different subsets of participants were asked either:

- About how fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other?

- About how fast were the cars going when they hit each other?

. . . or not asked about the cars' speed at all. A week later the group was asked if they had seen broken glass in addition to a list of other questions about the accident. The films did not show any glass breaking. The answers of the three groups varied according to how they were questioned a week earlier:



The study also showed how poorly people estimate speed regardless of the language that is used. Four films showed one collision at 20 mph; one at 30 mph; and two at 40 mph. The participants guessed the speed of the cars in each film to be between 35-40 mph.



When you’re setting up a remote war room and/or getting ready to work at the courthouse, don’t forget these essentials:


  1. A manual three-hole puncher - to use in the courtroom when binders need to be updated quietly.

  2. Litigation bags - so witness examination binders can be carried into court with style.

  3. A Bluebook. cite checking happens during trial.

  4. A ‘clicker’ remote control for PowerPoints.

  5. A binder containing trial exhibits for which a party has an objection.


Here's another tip from Bryan A. Garner's The Winning Brief: 100 Tips for Persuasive Briefing in Trial and Appellate Courts.


You should always capitalize 'court', when referring to:

  1. The United States Supreme Court.

  2. The highest court in the jurisdiction in which a filing is made.

  3. The court to which a filing is addressed.

Local rules will sometimes have additional rules about when to capitalize court . For example, the New York Law Reports Style Manual Rule 10.1(h) directs us to, "[c]apitalize the word 'court' when standing alone only when referring to the Supreme Court of the United States, the New York Court of Appeals or the Appellate Division of the Supreme Court."

Sean O'Shea has more than 20 years of experience in the litigation support field with major law firms in New York and San Francisco.   He is an ACEDS Certified eDiscovery Specialist and a Relativity Certified Administrator.

The views expressed in this blog are those of the owner and do not reflect the views or opinions of the owner’s employer.

If you have a question or comment about this blog, please make a submission using the form to the right. 

Your details were sent successfully!

© 2015 by Sean O'Shea . Proudly created with Wix.com

bottom of page